The Ears Of Your Fears

Imagine that you’re a young rat. You’re in the woods, doing rat things among the dead foliage. Suddenly, off to your left, you hear a sound that you've never heard before. It’s loud, it’s strange, and it’s… rattley.

Imagine that you’re a young rat. You’re in the woods, doing

rat things among the dead foliage. Suddenly, off to your left, you hear a sound

that you've never heard before. It’s loud, it’s strange, and it’s… rattley.

The brain has a few jobs to do in a short amount of time.

First it has to turn the sound into a neural stimulus, digesting the sound into

its component parts—frequency, loudness, harmonics, modulations and gaps—before

it can really be comprehended. Then, the signal has to be delivered to the

areas of the brain that will help identify the sound and determine if it’s a

threat. But it’s in a rat’s best interest to react first and think later.

Little woodland creatures have no time for contemplation. So your rat brain has

two parallel auditory pathways that diverge: one goes up to the analytical centers of the cortex while the other goes directly to the emotional response

regions that modulate feelings like fear.

The place where the pathways diverge, the medial geniculate

body, or MGB, used to be thought of as a routine stop on the way up to areas

that matter more. But recent work has shown that the region is much more

complex than originally thought. In fact, it turns out that the MGBm is a fine example of the immediate frontier of basic neuroscience. The lesser-known aspects of the MGB were the

subject of a symposium at the Association for Research in Otolaryngology

meeting in Baltimore this past weekend.

Model of pathways through the MGB

The MGB is a part of the thalamus, the central routing

station of the brain. The brain has two MGBs: a left and a right. If you cut

into the brain from ear to ear, the cross section of the MGB resembles two

round ears jutting out from the center core of the brain. The main division of

the MGB that sends up information to the primary auditory cortex is called the

ventral MGB (MGBv). The medial division (MGBm) is thinner, wrapping around the

MGBv like a shell. Its cells don’t project up to the auditory cortex. Instead,

they project to the amygdala, a region known for its involvement in emotional

processing. The MGBm is the door to the amygdala for auditory information, becoming, in a figurative sense, the "ears of our fears".

To understand how the MGBm processes signals, imagine that

you work on an assembly line. One person will make a component, the next person

will add on something, all the way down the line to you, and all the way down

to where the product is finished. The work is linear and predictable, and every

step makes some sense. Now, imagine that, instead of this stepwise procession,

every person on the assembly line take what they've been working on and dump

everything in your lap, and you are supposed to combine all of those pieces

together in a way that makes sense. And you don’t just get components from your

own assembly line.

That’s what the MGBm does. It receives direct input from

every level of the ascending auditory network, from the cochlear nucleus, where

the first digitized signals come in from the ears, to the processing centers

that individually encode the spatial and temporal features of the sound, to the

integrating centers that recombine all the digested factors of the sound for

higher-order processing, to the auditory cortex itself. All of that gets sent

directly to the MGBm directly. But that’s not all. It also gets input from the

superior colliculus, which is part of the visual system that coordinates eye

movements, and from regions of the spinal cord involved tactile senses.

It seems like the result would be a confusing, messy

conglomeration of data. And it is. Research has been lacking on MGBm neurons precisely

because their properties are frustratingly heterogenous. But the MGBm makes it

work, and scientists are getting just the first ideas about how.

Based on what we know, the consolidated MGBm signal seems to

be a kind of “significant event” detector. With its array of input, it can tell

if a sound is loud, close, and new. It can combine that information with visual

things caught by the eye, or raw input from its sense of touch, to come up with

a rough output that can tell the amygdala that something that should elicit

some kind of emotion is happening. So, the amygdala doesn’t have to wait for

the higher auditory cortex before it knows that something scary, like a

rattlesnake, is nearby—thanks to the MGBm, it can make that determination from

the raw data, and perhaps that will make the difference in saving its life.

Of course, this system is even more complex than I have

described. Not all of the regions of the MGBm overlap or connect to the same

individual neurons. And not all of the MGBm outputs go to the amygdala. The fine

details are still being worked out. In labs in Spain, Hong Kong, Britain and across the U.S., an increasing number of anatomists and physiologists are working to

find the fine details of how the MGBm works.

Why? Because it appears that the MGBm is a direct gateway

into our emotional response. So much of the way we respond to sounds around

us—a word in conversation, a bird song, a gunshot—depends on how our brains

interpret the sounds before we’re even aware that we’re hearing something. The

idea that the MGBm is the true “ear of our fears” is probably over-dramatic, but

nor is it unreasonable. 

Regions like the MGBm sit on the frontier of neuroscience.

We understand things that happen in the cochlea and retina really well. We can

tap into cells in the brainstem or cortex in certain places and understand

exactly what a given neuron is encoding: a sound, a shape, a movement, a

specific memory. But in the bizarre regions just outside of these more translatable

parts of the brain, we don’t really know what the neurons are doing. We can’t

speak its language yet. By delving into the workings of the MGBm, we understand

more about the mysteries of how the brain works at higher and higher levels.

In other words, studying the MGBm may help us better

understand the mechanisms of what makes us human, even if we have to learn the

lessons from rats.

For more:

Ledoux  1998: "Organization of projections to the lateral amygdala from auditory and visual areas of the thalamus in the rat."

Weinberger 2011: "The medial geniculate, not the amygdala, as the root of auditory fear conditioning."

Bartlett 2011: "Correlation of of neural response properties with auditory thalamus subdivisions in the awake marmoset."

Anderson 2009: "Stimulus specific adaptation occurs in the auditory thalamus."

Lee 2012: "Thalamic and cortical pathways supporting auditory processing."

Old NID
104281
Categories

Latest reads

Article teaser image
Donald Trump does not have the power to rescind either constitutional amendments or federal laws by mere executive order, no matter how strongly he might wish otherwise. No president of the United…
Article teaser image
The Biden administration recently issued a new report showing causal links between alcohol and cancer, and it's about time. The link has been long-known, but alcohol carcinogenic properties have been…
Article teaser image
In British Iron Age society, land was inherited through the female line and husbands moved to live with the wife’s community. Strong women like Margaret Thatcher resulted.That was inferred due to DNA…